The Predatory Baboon

By: Robert S. O. Harding

Originally Published in 1974

View PDF

Between September 1970 and October 1971, I made a field study of a troop of free-ranging olive baboons near the village of Gilgil in Kenya. I spent the first three months sur­veying baboon populations in the area, select­ing one troop for a detailed study, and getting that troop accustomed to my presence. In the last eleven months, I spent over one thousand hours observing these animals from as little as ten yards away. I had two objectives: to cor­relate the way in which the baboons used their range with differing ecological conditions, and to collect information on the behavior of adult male baboons.

A baboon, sitting in a tree, eating a small animal.
Harassed by other male baboons, Carl retreats to a tree branch to consume a small dikdik.

The troop I studied, one of at least seven living in an area of about 17 square miles. spent their days ranging across the parallel valleys of a large cattle ranch an the floor of the Rift Valley. These valleys, separated by tall cliffs up to 300 feet high, were mostly open grassland, interspersed with patches of scrub­by bush and stands of a large crown-sprouting shrub called leleshwa.

There have been humans in this part of Kenya for a long time: near the ranch, the Acheulian handaxes of Middle Pleistocene man have been found in quantity at Kariandusi, a site originally excavated by the late Dr. L. S. B. Leakey. More recently, twentieth-century man and his domestic livestock have had a significant effect on the habitat, Pred­ator control programs, undertaken to protect cattle, have presumably reduced the number of big cats in the area, although there are no records available to support this supposition. Lions are only occasional visitors, and leop­ards have been trapped periodically for re­moval to national parks, although they were known to exist in the baboons’ home range. Another major change has been the introduc­tion of water in cattle troughs throughout the ranch. Finally, large areas of leleshwa scrub have been cleared to make room for grassland.

Map of East Africa around the Equator.

These alterations in the habitat appar­ently have also caused local populations of hoofed animals to expand. Thomson’s gazelle, which prefer an open habitat and are capable of lambing twice a year, have greatly in­creased their numbers over the past fifteen years, keeping pace with the range-clearing program. The reduced number of predators may also have contributed to the increase in antelopes; at any rate, at the time of this study scientists from Texas A, and M. University estimated that 2,000 Thomson’s gazelle and 800 impala lived in the area which the baboon study troop used as its home range. Although human intervention in the recent past changed some crucial aspects of the Gilgil baboons’ habitat, at the same time they were relatively free from human harassment, since the coun­try on all sides was used for raising cattle and there were few human crops to serve as a point of conflict with local people. In addition, trapping of baboons for medical research, which has put heavy pressure on baboon pop­ulations elsewhere in Kenya, was not per­mitted on the ranch.

During the course of my study I saw ba­boons in my study troop catch, kill and eat an unusual number of small animals. This report will describe these events, compare them with similar behavior observed in other nonhuman primates, and discuss their implications for the origin of hunting in the Hominidae, the family that includes man and his immediate ancestors.

View of a grassy valley, mountains in the distance.
A grassy valley at Kekopey Ranch, in the Rift Valley of Kenya, typical habitat of the olive baboon.

At the outset it is important to understand that baboons, whatever their carnivorous propensities, subsist primarily on vegetable matter. My study of adult male baboons shows that these animals spent about 80% of their feeding time procuring and eating grass seeds, blades and roots, 18G/0 on fruits, seeds, and flowers, and only 2% on sources of animal protein, including beetles and other inverte­brates. Nonetheless, I saw the baboons of the study troop kill and eat 47 small animals in just over 1,000 hours of observation, a preda­tion rate far higher than has been reported for any other group of nonhuman primates. The chimpanzees studied by Dr. Jane van Lawick­Goodall and her associates at the Gombe National Park in Tanzania have killed and eaten more than 95 small animals, but these observations stretch over a decade.

At Gilgil, the preferred prey of the ba­boons was newborn Thomson’s gazelle in­fants. Sixteen of these animals were killed and eaten, and small antelope in general, including dikdik, impala, steinbok and others, made up two-thirds of the baboons’ prey. Twelve Cape hares, one button quail, and three small mam­mals I could not identify constituted the remaining third.

There was some evidence that local, learned tradition played a role in the selection of prey animals, since the baboons of the study troop did not try to catch and kill every small animal of a suitable size, and nearby troops were seen to eat entirely different prey animals. Rock hyrax, a small, furry animal looking rather like a badger, lived on most of the cliffs where the baboons went to sleep at night, and, although baboons and hyrax en­countered each other regularly, I never saw a baboon try to catch a hyrax. Subsequent ob­servers in Gilgil have evidence that other troops in the area may prey on hyrax, and elsewhere in East Africa this behavior has been reported as well.

A rocky cliff, vegetation growing scattered throughout the face.
Baboons retreat to cliffs such as these at night, to sleep huddled in rocky niches.

Flocks of helmeted guinea fowl were fre­quently seen wandering through the baboon troop as it sat eating grass, and, although guinea fowl were often within easy reach of the baboons, they were always ignored. How­ever, another large troop of baboons, whose home range overlapped that of the study troop, caught and ate guinea fowl on several occasions. It is thus clear that different troops have different notions as to what kind of an­imal is good to eat.

Adult female baboons were seen to catch only three of the forty-seven small animals which the troop killed, and on two of these three occasions the female was chased by an adult male and dropped her prey before she could eat it. The third female was also chased by a male, but her pursuer was distracted when she dropped part of her kill, and she was able to hide behind a bush and finish the rest.

The newborn antelopes, hares and other animals that the baboons killed and ate were all animals that defend themselves against predators by remaining immobile, using avail­able cover to conceal themselves. It is not surprising, therefore, that most kills were made by chance, when a male baboon tripped over a crouching hare, for instance. In one typical example, I saw the baboons move in to feed in a grassy field which had been vacated.

Two gazelles locking antlers.
Male Thomson’s gazelle in a territorial dispute. Infants of this pecies of antelope were a favorite baboon prey.

Yet, killings were not always fortuitous; several times I watched male baboons leave the troop as it made its way across an open field, move over to a herd of grazing antelope, and begin to walk a criss-cross pattern through the herd, eyes directed at the grass on either side. The male baboons also made sim­ilar excursions away from the troop into brushy areas, typical habitat of the dikdik, a tiny, fox-terrier-sized antelope. Both types of be­havior appeared to be conscious efforts to look for suitable prey, and each resulted in a kill on occasion. Although the male baboons often took up the chase of a prey animal al­ready being pursued by another baboon, there was no indication that the monkeys were co­operating with each other by doing so.

Once the prey animal had been caught, the baboons made no deliberate attempts to kill before beginning to eat it, just as many carnivores do not kill their prey right away but begin to eat it as soon as it is under their control. In the case of newborn antelope, the baboons usually began by eating the soft underside of the animal, and death quickly followed. Smaller animals, such as the button quail, virtually disappeared in a mouthful, but young antelope were carefully eaten. The ba­boons tore off chunks of meat by gripping the flesh with their front teeth, grasping their prey with two or more limbs, and pulling back their heads. A baboon often stripped the skin from an antelope limb with its front teeth, but in general the meat was torn from the inside of the skin and no attempt was made to skin it before eating it. I examined almost every kill site carefully after the baboons had finished eating and found that very little had been left uneaten; usually only scraps of skin or an occasional long bone remained. As a final morsel, the baboons often punctured the skull with their teeth and consumed the brain of their prey.

Once a prey animal had been captured, others of its species usually did little except to stand at a distance and watch it being eaten; however, several times female antelope were seen trying to rescue their young. On one of these occasions a female impala successfully defended her infant, apparently unable to get to its feet, from repeated attacks from at least two adult male baboons. In the course of chas­ing the baboons to their sleeping cliffs she managed to incite other impala herds in the same valley, first an all-male herd of fifteen animals and then a “harem” herd of one male and twenty-three females, with the result that



A small antelope.
The klipspringer, a small antelope with hooves like hard rubber which enable it to live in the near-vertical habitat of rocky cliffs, much like the chamois. Klipspringer young were also taken by baboons.



A baby antelope in tall grass.
Baboons combed brushy thickets in search of immature dikdik, antelope which weight only 10-12 pounds when fully adult.


As already mentioned, adult female, juvenile and infant baboons were virtually excluded from meat-eating during the year I spent in Gilgil. There was an unequal distribu­tion of meat among the adult males as well, however. Adult male Carl, who was the largest and most influential animal in the troop, caught and ate eighteen small animals, almost twice the number caught by the next most accomplished male, Sumner, who caught ten. Males Radcliffe, Alger and Moses caught five, three, and two animals respectively, while three were caught by females and six by males I could not identify at the time. These figures are deceptive, however, since Carl was able to add considerably to the amount of meat he ate by confiscating animals killed by other baboons immediately after they had been taken. Carl obtained an additional nine an­imals this way, making him the prime meat consumer of the troop, with twenty-seven an­imals eaten, or 57% of those caught. No other baboon came close to Carl in this regard, and Moses, at the other end of the scale, was forced to give up his two captures to Carl and finished with no animals to his credit. How­ever, Moses, like other males, was often able to acquire some meat by being the first to grab the carcass after the original eater had aban­doned it, and in this way he was still able to get more meat than any female.

I never saw a baboon share meat volun­tarily with any other baboon, unless leaving the carcass on the ground when satiated can be called a form of sharing. On the contrary, a kill generally caused aggression levels in the troop to rise markedly. The most common reaction of the males to a kill by one of their number was to sit like a vulture within five yards of the meat-eater and stare at him, occa­sionally threatening him and any other males nearby. While relative status in the troop sometimes appeared to determine which ba­boon got most of a prey animal, this was not always the case. For instance, adult male Carl was often seen watching a male of lower status eat meat without any overt attempt to deprive him of it. In a similar incident, Rad­cliffe was able to drive Carl from the carcass of an infant gazelle, even though at the time Radcliffe was wounded and had become the least influential male in the troop. Thus, male status was not always a reliable predictor of behavior around a kill.

A baboon seated in tall grass, eating a baby gazelle.
Adult male baboon Radcliffe has just stumbled upon a newborn Thomson’s gazelle, and tears chunks of flesh from its undersides with his front teeth.

Baboons passed up carrion and ate meat only when either they or another baboon of the same troop had recently killed the prey animal. Thus the study troop ignored the car­cass of a steinbok that had died less than 24 hours earlier, although several baboons passed within a yard of it during a troop movement. Similarly, a juvenile baboon was seen driving a large Verreaux’s eagle from the remains of a newborn mountain reedbuck, and, although the juvenile and several other baboons pawed at the dead antelope, they made no attempt to eat it.

Similarly, a juvenile baboon was seen driving a large Verreaux’s eagle from the remains of a newborn mountain reedbuck, and, although the juvenile and several other baboons pawed at the dead antelope, they made no attempt to eat it.

I could not watch every meat-eating bout until the last scrap of hide had been chewed by the last baboon involved, but most prob­ably lasted less than an hour, with a hare tak­ing about fifteen minutes to dispose of and the larger antelopes proportionately longer. The longest meat-eating session I saw lasted just under two hours, in part because of a complex set of aggressive interactions which resulted in four males and several other animals gain­ing access to the meat.

A baboon seated in tall grass, using its teeth to skin a small gazelle.
Radcliffe strips the skin from the forelimb of the dead gazelle.

There seemed to be no correlation be­tween external factors and the killing of small animals by baboons. For example, rainfall is seasonal in Gilgil, and I thought it likely that killing would increase during the dry season, when other sources of baboon food were rela­tively scarce and there was little cover avail­able for prey animals. This did not happen, however. Similarly, Thomson’s gazelle are known to have birth peaks in other parts of East Africa, but, in contrast to what I might have predicted, there were no corresponding peaks in the killing and eating of newborn gazelles. The troop moved significantly farther during the hours beginning with 10:30 a.m. and 3:30 p.m. than at any other time, and this increased movement should have resulted in more chance discovery and killing of small animals. It did not. Finally, there were some occasions when the baboons seemed to kill several animals in a short period, but there were not enough examples of this clustering in time to prove that a successful kill was a major stimulus to additional killing.

Many nonhuman primates thrive in cap­tivity on diets that include meat, and the fact that baboons kill and eat small animals in the wild has been known for some time. The South African anthropologist Raymond Dart has compiled a record of references in the early literature to the predatory habits of ba­boons, combining reports of attacks upon South African domestic stock by baboons with incidents in which baboons were seen to kill wild game. Dart’s purpose in collecting this sort of information was to show, in his words, “that predatory behaviour in a large terrestrial primate other than man was nat­ural, and that such behaviour was consistent with the insectivorous origin and diet of pri­mates.” If baboons could be shown to have a “perpetual” and “instinctive need” for animal protein, as Dart felt they had, then an equally good case could be made for Australopithecus, a primitive hominid first discovered by Dart. More recent field studies of baboons by differ­ent observers in different parts of Africa have confirmed that these monkeys in the wild will occasionally kill and eat small animals.

A mother impala charging a baboon that holds a baby impala, another baboon watches.
A female impala charges male baboon Carl (right) in a vain attempt to rescue her infant which he holds in his teeth.

The chimpanzee is the only other non­human primate to regularly include small mammals in its diet. In the first ten years spent observing the chimpanzees that fre­quent the van Lawick-Goodall’s research sta­tion in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania, 95 animals were known to have been killed by chimpanzees and another 37 unsuccessful predatory attempts recorded. 65% of the prey animals were other primates, such as baboon infants, red colobus, blue monkey and red-tailed monkey, while bushbuck and bushpig accounted for the remainder. Japanese observers in Uganda have also seen chimpanzees eating meat on a small scale, and recent pub­lished reports describe two examples of chim­panzee cannibalism, one definite case in the Gombe National Park and one probable report from Uganda.

In both the baboons I studied and van Lawick-Goodall’s chimpanzees, the killing of small animals appeared to be an activity car­ried on only by adults and almost exclusively by males. In contrast to the Gilgil baboons, however, the Gombe chimpanzees cooperated with one another in obtaining a prey animal: several chimpanzees have been seen to block the escape routes of a young baboon while one of their number advanced slowly on the intended victim. Chimpanzees have also deliberately wrung or bitten into the neck of their prey before beginning to eat it, whereas I never saw the Gilgil baboons do this.

A mother antelope and two adolescent antelope standing in grass.
Unable to save her infant, the mother impala and two younger antelope watch the small antelope being eaten.

The Gombe chimpanzees seemed to treat a prey animal as common property for the first few minutes after a kill, and interference with any other ape trying to obtain the meat was rare. After several minutes had gone by, however, the chimpanzees with the major portion of the meat established some priority over it, and others had to beg for a share. Nonetheless, sharing almost always occurred. In ten years of observation, only two unshared kills were seen, and these were the simultaneous killings of two small bushpigs by two adult female chimpanzees when no others were present. When a Gilgil baboon killed a small animal, however, intense aggression often occurred, in which case competition over the prey animal sprang up quickly. Some of this activity resulted in depriving the captor of his prey, but a baboon was never seen to share meat voluntarily. This is in sharp contrast to the observations of Geza Teleki, who has made a special study of meat-eating in chimpanzees: in 43 hours spent watching meat being shared at the Gombe Stream, chimpanzees were never seen to fight for possession of it.

The Gombe chimpanzees also spent a great deal of time eating their prey. Teleki watched one meat-eating bout that lasted nine hours, and states that the mean duration of the predatory episodes he personally witnessed was just under four hours. The shortest episode was one hour and three-quarters, roughly the same amount of time as the longest feeding bout of the Gilgil baboons. At least part of the difference in time taken to eat a prey animal may have been the result of the Gombe chimpanzees’ habit of alternating mouthfuls of leaves with mouthfuls of meat, a habit the Gilgil baboons did not share. At the same time, the relatively tense atmosphere prevailing around a baboon kill encouraged the bolting of meat, while the relaxed chimpanzees were free to eat their prey at whatever rate they chose.

A large adult male baboon.
Adult male baboon Carl, who killed and ate more small animals than any other baboon in his troop during 1970-71.

No complete analysis has yet been made of the protein content of baboon or chimpanzee diets in the wild, but it is apparent that protein from freshly-killed animals is a minor part of their overall food consumption. Moreover, in spite of the amount of predation seen in this one troop of baboons, it is worth emphasizing that adult females, juveniles and infants obtained very little meat while adult males spent only two per cent of their feeding time eating protein from animal sources. Meat was more widely distributed among the Gombe chimpanzees, but it still remained a relatively unimportant part of their diet. It is therefore highly unlikely that baboons, at least, have a “pressing,” “instinctive” need for animal protein, as Raymond Dart suggested.

At the same time, it is interesting to note that at least two species of nonhuman primate are capable of catching and consuming sub­stantial numbers of small animals without any particular anatomical specialization. These animals have brains that make them capable of cooperative hunting, locomotor anatomy that enables them to catch small prey, teeth able to prepare animal food for ingestion, and a digestive apparatus well-equipped to make use of animal matter, including small amounts of bone. I know of nothing to indicate that these are recent evolutionary developments; indeed, while Dart’s contention that man’s im­mediate nonhuman primate ancestors were insectivores has not been proven, it is cer­tainly likely that they were omnivores who made small animals, and especially other pri­mates, a regular part of their diet.

An adult male baboon sitting in a tree, yawning.
Adult male Carl, yawning as the tension around an antelope kill rises, displays the long razor-sharp canine teeth which are a male baboon’s most formidable weapons.

Many of the recent books emphasizing the biological basis of human behavior have isolated the beginning of hunting as a key development in human evolution, and the pic­ture many of us retain is that of the Australo­pithecus-like creature in the film 2001—A Space Odyssey raising a bone and smashing it downward in dramatic slow motion. Some writers have even equated early meat-eating hominids with the Biblical story of Cain, add­ing what is surely unnecessary emotional freight to the whole issue. Human aggression is often traced back to the development of a propensity to kill other animals for food, and from there it is but a short step to man’s inhu­manity to man in general and war in particular.

That some of us may kill our fellow men because our ancestors learned to kill other animals seems even less likely in the light of recent research in animal behavior. Experi­ments with cats, for instance, have shown that prey-catching and aggression in these animals are separate behavioral systems controlled by different parts of the brain, while the increas­ing number of field studies of wild animal populations has produced a number of exam­ples of other creatures, carnivores and other­wise, which kill their own kind.

A baboon skinning a small animal with its teeth.
Baboons normally ate meat by stripping it off the inside of the hide with their front teeth.

The evidence presented here, then, sup­ports the notion that the development of predatory behavior may not have been the major shaping force in human evolution that some have called it. Instead, the diet of most of today’s human populations, a combination of animal and vegetable foods, is probably a continuation of an ancient primate pattern.

The Gilgil baboons have been followed without interruption by a series of observers since my study ended, and I plan to continue these observations with the assistance of stu­dents from the University of Pennsylvania for as long as possible. As our knowledge of these omnivorous baboons increases, one of our objectives will be to gain more insight into the origin and development of hunting and its importance in man’s evolution.

Cite This Article

Harding, Robert S. O.. "The Predatory Baboon." Expedition Magazine 16, no. 2 (January, 1974): -. Accessed April 24, 2024. https://www.penn.museum/sites/expedition/the-predatory-baboon/


This digitized article is presented here as a historical reference and may not reflect the current views of the Penn Museum.

Report problems and issues to digitalmedia@pennmuseum.org.